麻豆视频

Research

Connection speed in the brain may impact how people with autism process visual illusions

Jul. 22, 2024

Visual illusions continue to provide clues to how the brain processes what people with autism see. New research suggests that connections in the brain that send information about the context of what is being seen may operate more slowly in people with autism. 鈥淚n that case, it would make sense for the brain to focus more on the details of what we are seeing than on the big picture,鈥 said Emily Knight, MD, PhD, assistant professor of Neuroscience and Pediatrics and first author of the study published in the . 鈥淪ome of the things we would like to explore next are how these changes in timing might relate to the sensory or repetitive behaviors that we often see in individuals on the autism spectrum.鈥

As a clinician scientist, 碍苍颈驳丑迟鈥檚 research informs her work with patients and their families. 鈥淚t鈥檚 important to remember that the basic processing of what we see, hear, and touch forms the foundation for how we interact with our environment and people,鈥 Knight said. 鈥淟earning about how the brains of children who are neurodiverse may truly be seeing the world in a different way helps me to better understand and support them in our community.鈥

Additional authors include senior author John Foxe, PhD, director of the Golisano Institute for Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and the Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience and Edward Freedman, PhD, of the University of Rochester, Ted Altschuler, PhD, Sophie Molholm, PhD, and Jeremy Murphy of Albert Einstein College of Medicine. This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, the Rose F. Kennedy Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center, the University of Rochester Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center, and University of Rochester Clinical and Translational Science Institute KL2 Career Development Award.


Originally published on Feb. 28, 2023.

There is this picture 鈥 you may have seen it. It is black and white and has two silhouettes facing one another. Or maybe you see the black vase with a white background. But now, you likely see both.

Visual illusion two silluettes in white are facing eachother the negative space between them creates a vase.

It is an example of a visual illusion that reminds us to consider what we did not see at first glance, what we may not be able to see, or what our experience has taught us to know 鈥 there is always more to the picture or maybe even a different image to consider altogether. Researchers are finding the process in our brain that allows us to see these visual distinctions may not be happening the same way in the brains of children with autism spectrum disorder. They may be seeing these illusions differently.

鈥淗ow our brain puts together pieces of an object or visual scene is important in helping us interact with our environments,鈥 said Emily Knight, MD, PhD, assistant professor of Neuroscience and Pediatrics at the 麻豆视频, and first author on a study out today in the . 鈥淲hen we view an object or picture, our brains use processes that consider our experience and contextual information to help anticipate sensory inputs, address ambiguity, and fill in the missing information.鈥

Watching the brain 鈥榮ee鈥

Head and shoulders shot of Emily Knight, PhD, she has straight brown hair, brown eyes, and is smiling.
Emily Knight, MD, PhD

Knight and fellow neuroscience researchers in the Frederick J. and Marion A. Schindler Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory at the Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience used visual illusions 鈥 groups of Pac-Man-shaped images that create the illusion of a shape in the empty space. They worked with 60 children ages seven to 17 with and without autism. Using electroencephalography (EEG) 鈥 a non-invasive neuroimaging technique that allows researchers to record the response of neurons in the brain 鈥 researchers revealed that children with autism did not automatically process the illusory shapes as well as children without autism. It suggests that something is going awry in the feedback processing pathways in their brain.

鈥淭his tells us that these children may not be able to do the same predicting and filling in of missing visual information as their peers,鈥 Knight said. 鈥淲e now need to understand how this may relate to the atypical visual sensory behaviors we see in some children on the autism spectrum.鈥

碍苍颈驳丑迟鈥檚 past research, published in , found that children with autism may not be able to see or process body language like their peers, especially when distracted by something else. The kids in this study watched videos of dots that moved to represent a person. As part of the experiment, the dots changed color. Unlike in typical development, the brains of children with autism did not appear to notice the human movement when told to focus on the color. They had to pay specific attention to the human movement for their brains to process it well.

鈥淲e also need to continue this work with people on the autism spectrum who have a wider range of verbal and cognitive abilities and with other diagnoses such as ADHD,鈥 Knight said. 鈥淐ontinuing to use these neuroscientific tools, we hope to understand better how people with autism see the world so that we can find new ways to support children and adults on the autism spectrum.鈥

Collaboration aims to reveal more about neurodevelopmental diseases

Tackling complex neurodevelopmental diseases, such as autism, requires collaboration. The University of Rochester is one of about a dozen Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Centers (IDDRC) designated by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) as a national leader in research for conditions such as autism, Batten disease, and Rett syndrome. Knight and fellow researchers in both studies collaborated with fellow IDD researchers at the (RFK-IDDRC) at Albert Einstein College of Medicine.

John is wearing a blue suit coat, his arms are crossed and he has a slight smile.
John Foxe, PhD

鈥淎s scientists, we always need to pace ourselves. Research is a marathon 鈥 not a sprint. But being able to collaborate affords us time, space, and materials we may not otherwise have access to,鈥 said John Foxe, PhD, senior author on both studies and co-director of the UR-IDDRC. 鈥淭his recent work is a wonderful example of this. We can increase our pace in the marathon by collaborating with others on the IDDRC team.鈥

Other authors of the Journal for Neuroscience paper include Ed Freedman, PhD, Evan Myers, PhD, Leona Oakes, PhD, of the University of Rochester, Alaina Berruti and Sophie Molholm, PhD, of Albert Einstein College of Medicine, and Cody Cao of the University of Michigan. This research was supported by the Kilian J. and Caroline F. Schmitt Foundation through Del Monte Institute for Neuroscience Pilot Program. The UR-IDDRC, RFK-IDDRC, and the 麻豆视频 Department of Pediatrics Chair Fellow Award, Kyle Family Fellowship, Visual Sciences at the University of Rochester post鈥恉octoral training fellowship.